There’s a remarkably simple notation for sketching cities, and I think it points at a better way to design software.
A notation for describing a city
Kevin Lynch was an urban planner who carried out pioneering work on people’s urban cognitive maps from the 1950s.
And:
As a planner, Lynch was interested in analysing the urban form, and in particular identified the criterion of the ‘legibility’ of a cityscape which he defined as ” the ease with which its parts can be recognized and can be organized into a coherent pattern”
… His method involved externalising the ‘mental images’ that city-dwellers have of their cities, through interviews and sketch-mapping exercises.
(From these lecture notes on the work of Kevin Lynch.)
This shared “mental images” is the subject of Lynch’s book The Image of City (1960) (and it blew my mind when I read it in - checks notes - 2003).
Here’s an example of one of Lynch’s maps: Boston.
What you’ll see from that map is that it’s totally recognisable as a city, and you could totally use it to navigate, but it’s also what you would scribble on the back of a napkin. It’s also way more memorable. If you gave me a glimpse of Boston from Google Maps and asked me to sketch it for someone else, I can’t imagine it would include any of the salient details. But given a Lynch map, I bet I could pass on the most relevant bare bones, just from memory.
So Lynch has managed to capture what is essential about maps for (a) understanding, and (b) communication.
Lynch’s insight is that these scribbled maps use a notation of only five elements. From those earlier lecture notes, there are:
Paths: They may be streets, walkways, transit lines, canals, railroads.
Edges: They are the boundaries … shores, railroad cuts, edges of development, walls.
Districts: Districts are the medium-to-large sections of the city … which the observer mentally enters “inside of”.
Nodes: They may be primarily junctions, places of a break in transportation, a crossing or convergence of paths … [or] a street-corner or an enclosed square.
Landmarks: They are usually a rather simply defined physical object: building, sign, store, or mountain. Their use involves the singling out of one element from a host of possibilities.
Out of these five elements, you can build an “image” (in Lynch’s terminology) of the city.
Do Lynch’s elements have a neurological underpinning?
Landmarks grab my attention, for this reason: they come up in Mind Hacks, in a chapter about memory and the hippocampus.
We know that the human brain has specialized mechanisms dedicated to remembering landmarks, and that (interestingly) this region and those nearby seem to be responsible for giving humans and other animals a sense of where they are in space. Brain images of people navigating through virtual environments has shown that even if we don’t consciously recognize something as a landmark it still triggers a response in this specialized part of the brain.
(Quick plug: Mind Hacks is now available in Chinese! Check out the 11 second product video with perky music on that page. That brings us up to 7 translated editions, which feels pretty special.)
So what I find intriguing is that we, us humans, appear to have a “landmark sense” that we all share.
Which is why, I guess, you can follow directions to go down the street and turn left at the fountain, and if you pass a cathedral then you know you’ve gone the wrong way – because such a landmark would certainly have been mentioned.
The question is this:
Do Lynch’s other elements also have neurological underpinnings?
And a follow up: If so, how could that be useful?
Your memory resets when you walk through a door
The reason I ask is because of the Doorway Effect, which is something that happens also in physical space and not outdoors but indoors: Memory was worse after passing through a doorway than after walking the same distance within a single room.
… some forms of memory seem to be optimized to keep information ready-to-hand until its shelf life expires, and then purge that information in favor of new stuff. Radvansky and colleagues call this sort of memory representation an “event model,” and propose that walking through a doorway is a good time to purge your event models because whatever happened in the old room is likely to become less relevant now that you have changed venues.
What’s especially intriguing about this study…
The Doorway Effect appears for real doorways. But ALSO: It doesn’t seem to matter, for instance, whether the virtual environments are displayed on a 66” flat screen or a 17” CRT.
So, to review the precarious stack of speculation that I’m on:
there are shared neurological underpinnings for how we understand space, and - in theory - an abstract system of space “elements”
in at least one situation, there is a correlation between how we move through space and how we mentally organise information
“moving through space” doesn’t need to be physical, but can be triggered by abstract representations.
Which provokes two thoughts:
If landmarks and doorways, then what is the full set of elements, and are there automatic memory operations for all of them?
How abstract can these spatial representations be, and can we use them in software?
What should Wikipedia or Notion do?
Given there’s an explosion in software to accrete and organise knowledge, is the page model really the best approach?
Perhaps the building blocks shouldn’t be pages or blocks, but
neighbourhoods
roads
rooms and doors
landmarks.
Or rather, as a knowledge base or wiki develops, it should - just like a real city - encourage its users to gravitate towards these different fundamental elements. A page that starts to function a little bit like a road should transform into a slick navigation element, available on all its linked pages. A page which is functioning like a landmark should start being visible from two hops away.
It would be interesting to investigate exactly what the minimal level of physical appearance is required to trigger the automatic behaviour of loading/resetting human memory and associations.
Like, following a hyperlink might not activate the neurological automation.
But what if there was a zooming out animation, or a change in colour, or the old page slid off to the side?
What’s the minimum you need to trick your brain into believing that you’re moving around an environment?
And could design features as simple as these make tools like Notion, Roam, Obsidian, Evernote and other note taking software, Wikipedia, etc, radically better for organising, navigating, sharing, and internalising knowledge, for individuals and for teams? If so, can you imagine the efficiency gains and the new ideas that could emerge?
Hippocampus ergonomics.
It would be worth a research lab and a year or two, I think.
One final and quite literal idea: Could Lynch-style maps be generated automatically, and could this be an interface to Google Maps?
This paper believes this is possible: A computational approach to ‘The Image of the City’.
Although: Out of the five elements, landmarks were found most challenging to extract.
My guess is that landmarks can’t be extracted from maps because they’re reliant on the visual field, approaches, other nearby potential landmarks, and so on. You would need to train an artificial landmark sensor in a machine hippocampus, perhaps giving it access to Street View and getting feedback data from humans on the spot asked to point at their nearest landmark.
Computationally producing Lynch maps would also allow for the reverse process, which is to give a robot car directions in the same way you would a person: down the street, left at the big building, follow it till the end and we’re the second on the left.
‘Yes, we’ll see them together some Saturday afternoon then,’ she said. ‘I won’t have any hand in your not going to Cathedral on Sunday morning. I suppose we must be getting back. What time was it when you looked at your watch just now?’ "In China and some other countries it is not considered necessary to give the girls any education; but in Japan it is not so. The girls are educated here, though not so much as the boys; and of late years they have established schools where they receive what we call the higher branches of instruction. Every year new schools for girls are opened; and a great many of the Japanese who formerly would not be seen in public with their wives have adopted the Western idea, and bring their wives into society. The marriage laws have been arranged so as to allow the different classes to marry among[Pg 258] each other, and the government is doing all it can to improve the condition of the women. They were better off before than the women of any other Eastern country; and if things go on as they are now going, they will be still better in a few years. The world moves. "Frank and Fred." She whispered something to herself in horrified dismay; but then she looked at me with her eyes very blue and said "You'll see him about it, won't you? You must help unravel this tangle, Richard; and if you do I'll--I'll dance at your wedding; yours and--somebody's we know!" Her eyes began forewith. Lawrence laughed silently. He seemed to be intensely amused about something. He took a flat brown paper parcel from his pocket. making a notable addition to American literature. I did truly. "Surely," said the minister, "surely." There might have been men who would have remembered that Mrs. Lawton was a tough woman, even for a mining town, and who would in the names of their own wives have refused to let her cross the threshold of their homes. But he saw that she was ill, and he did not so much as hesitate. "I feel awful sorry for you sir," said the Lieutenant, much moved. "And if I had it in my power you should go. But I have got my orders, and I must obey them. I musn't allow anybody not actually be longing to the army to pass on across the river on the train." "Throw a piece o' that fat pine on the fire. Shorty," said the Deacon, "and let's see what I've got." "Further admonitions," continued the Lieutenant, "had the same result, and I was about to call a guard to put him under arrest, when I happened to notice a pair of field-glasses that the prisoner had picked up, and was evidently intending to appropriate to his own use, and not account for them. This was confirmed by his approaching me in a menacing manner, insolently demanding their return, and threatening me in a loud voice if I did not give them up, which I properly refused to do, and ordered a Sergeant who had come up to seize and buck-and-gag him. The Sergeant, against whom I shall appear later, did not obey my orders, but seemed to abet his companion's gross insubordination. The scene finally culminated, in the presence of a number of enlisted men, in the prisoner's wrenching the field-glasses away from me by main force, and would have struck me had not the Sergeant prevented this. It was such an act as in any other army in the world would have subjected the offender to instant execution. It was only possible in—" "Don't soft-soap me," the old woman snapped. "I'm too old for it and I'm too tough for it. I want to look at some facts, and I want you to look at them, too." She paused, and nobody said a word. "I want to start with a simple statement. We're in trouble." RE: Fruyling's World "MACDONALD'S GATE" "Read me some of it." "Well, I want something better than that." HoME大香蕉第一时间
ENTER NUMBET 0016khizeu.com.cn lucoqn.com.cn www.lnsncp.com.cn www.letubd.com.cn www.jtzher.com.cn www.jcchdt.com.cn ifgcud.com.cn e-ting.net.cn supernao.com.cn nschain.com.cn
There’s a remarkably simple notation for sketching cities, and I think it points at a better way to design software.
A notation for describing a city
Kevin Lynch was
And:
(From these lecture notes on the work of Kevin Lynch.)
This shared “mental images” is the subject of Lynch’s book The Image of City (1960) (and it blew my mind when I read it in - checks notes - 2003).
Here’s an example of one of Lynch’s maps: Boston.
What you’ll see from that map is that it’s totally recognisable as a city, and you could totally use it to navigate, but it’s also what you would scribble on the back of a napkin. It’s also way more memorable. If you gave me a glimpse of Boston from Google Maps and asked me to sketch it for someone else, I can’t imagine it would include any of the salient details. But given a Lynch map, I bet I could pass on the most relevant bare bones, just from memory.
So Lynch has managed to capture what is essential about maps for (a) understanding, and (b) communication.
Lynch’s insight is that these scribbled maps use a notation of only five elements. From those earlier lecture notes, there are:
Out of these five elements, you can build an “image” (in Lynch’s terminology) of the city.
Do Lynch’s elements have a neurological underpinning?
Landmarks grab my attention, for this reason: they come up in Mind Hacks, in a chapter about memory and the hippocampus.
(Quick plug: Mind Hacks is now available in Chinese! Check out the 11 second product video with perky music on that page. That brings us up to 7 translated editions, which feels pretty special.)
So what I find intriguing is that we, us humans, appear to have a “landmark sense” that we all share.
Which is why, I guess, you can follow directions to go down the street and turn left at the fountain, and if you pass a cathedral then you know you’ve gone the wrong way – because such a landmark would certainly have been mentioned.
The question is this:
Do Lynch’s other elements also have neurological underpinnings?
And a follow up: If so, how could that be useful?
Your memory resets when you walk through a door
The reason I ask is because of the Doorway Effect, which is something that happens also in physical space and not outdoors but indoors:
What’s especially intriguing about this study…
The Doorway Effect appears for real doorways. But ALSO:
So, to review the precarious stack of speculation that I’m on:
Which provokes two thoughts:
What should Wikipedia or Notion do?
Given there’s an explosion in software to accrete and organise knowledge, is the page model really the best approach?
Perhaps the building blocks shouldn’t be pages or blocks, but
Or rather, as a knowledge base or wiki develops, it should - just like a real city - encourage its users to gravitate towards these different fundamental elements. A page that starts to function a little bit like a road should transform into a slick navigation element, available on all its linked pages. A page which is functioning like a landmark should start being visible from two hops away.
It would be interesting to investigate exactly what the minimal level of physical appearance is required to trigger the automatic behaviour of loading/resetting human memory and associations.
Like, following a hyperlink might not activate the neurological automation.
But what if there was a zooming out animation, or a change in colour, or the old page slid off to the side?
What’s the minimum you need to trick your brain into believing that you’re moving around an environment?
And could design features as simple as these make tools like Notion, Roam, Obsidian, Evernote and other note taking software, Wikipedia, etc, radically better for organising, navigating, sharing, and internalising knowledge, for individuals and for teams? If so, can you imagine the efficiency gains and the new ideas that could emerge?
Hippocampus ergonomics.
It would be worth a research lab and a year or two, I think.
One final and quite literal idea: Could Lynch-style maps be generated automatically, and could this be an interface to Google Maps?
This paper believes this is possible: A computational approach to ‘The Image of the City’.
Although:
My guess is that landmarks can’t be extracted from maps because they’re reliant on the visual field, approaches, other nearby potential landmarks, and so on. You would need to train an artificial landmark sensor in a machine hippocampus, perhaps giving it access to Street View and getting feedback data from humans on the spot asked to point at their nearest landmark.
Computationally producing Lynch maps would also allow for the reverse process, which is to give a robot car directions in the same way you would a person: down the street, left at the big building, follow it till the end and we’re the second on the left.